Infants recognize foreign languages as a form of communication

infant
Credit: CC0 Public Domain

Infants recognize that speech in a language not their own is used for communication, finds a new psychology study. The results, which appear in the journal Cognition, offer new insights into how language is processed at a young age.

"By their first birthday, babies understand that foreign languages can communicate information between people, even though the babies themselves don't understand the foreign ," explains Athena Vouloumanos, an associate professor in NYU's Department of Psychology and the author of the study. "This tells us that ' processing of formal aspects of language, such as speech sounds and , is separate from infants' processing of communicative aspects of language—specifically, the transferring of information."

It has long been established that infants understand that speech in their native language allows speakers to communicate. Less clear is whether or not this understanding limited to their native language—does it extend to non-native languages with which infants have no experience?

To address this question, Vouloumanos conducted a series of experiments in which 12-month-old infants viewed human actors communicating in multiple ways. The infants' responses—i.e., their gaze—were recorded by an observer. Gaze length is a commonly used measurement for spotting infants' comprehension of language and concepts.

In the experiments, the infants saw an actor, the Communicator, repeatedly select one of two objects. When the Communicator could no longer reach the target but a second player, a Recipient, could, the Communicator vocalized a nonsense phrase either in English (infants' native language), Spanish (rhythmically different), or Russian (structurally different), or hummed (a non-speech vocalization). The infants had not been exposed to Spanish or Russian before.

Across all three languages, native and non-native, but not humming, infants looked longer when the Recipient gave the Communicator the non-target compared to the target object. By contrast, when the Communicator hummed, infants looked equally at the non-target and target outcomes. By looking longer when the Recipient handed over the non-target object, the infants seemed to recognize that a miscommunication had taken place or that the verbal interaction had some communicative value—even if it wasn't in their native language.

"These results indicate that infants can generalize beyond their specific experience with their own to recognize that all languages can allow people to communicate," Vouloumanos explains. "By 12 months, infants do not readily map non-native words to objects or discriminate most non-native speech contrasts, but they can understand that non-native languages can transfer information to others."

More information: Athena Vouloumanos, Voulez-vous jouer avec moi? Twelve-month-olds understand that foreign languages can communicate, Cognition (2018). DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.01.002

Journal information: Cognition
Citation: Infants recognize foreign languages as a form of communication (2018, January 24) retrieved 29 March 2024 from https://medicalxpress.com/news/2018-01-infants-foreign-languages.html
This document is subject to copyright. Apart from any fair dealing for the purpose of private study or research, no part may be reproduced without the written permission. The content is provided for information purposes only.

Explore further

Do infants judge others' language proficiency? It depends on their own, research shows

1069 shares

Feedback to editors