Optical illusions reveal that regular waves of brain activity enable visual feature integration

Optical illusions reveal regular waves of brain activity enable visual feature integration
Any two of these images would flash briefly onscreen during the visual perception tests performed by University of Tokyo researchers. The images would appear some unpredictable time after volunteers pushed a button to start the test. Viewers had to identify both the light intensity (white or black color) and the orientation of the image (left or right diagonal lines). Credit: CC-BY-ND Nakayama and Motoyoshi 2019, originally published in The Journal of Neuroscience

Rhythmic waves of brain activity cause people to see or not see complex images that flash before our eyes. An image can become practically invisible if it flashes before our eyes at the same time as a low point of those brain waves. We can reset that brain wave rhythm with a simple voluntary action, like choosing to push a button.

The new results come from experts who use to understand human vision. Human sight involves activity both in the eyes and in the . Optical illusions are formed by playing tricks on any part of our complex visual system.

"This is the first record of rhythmic brain activity used to achieve integrated ," said Associate Professor Isamu Motoyoshi from the University of Tokyo and co-author of the recent research article.

Reoccurring rhythms of attention and perception

Previous research identified that attention fluctuates six to eight times per second (6 to 8 Hertz), a rate researchers refer to as theta rhythms.

The recent investigation started when Motoyoshi and colleagues were studying the Gabor pattern illusion, in which a smoothly moving circle appears to jump across the screen.

Viewers saw jumps at a constant regardless of the actual speed of the illusion. The rhythm of jumps in the optical illusion was remarkably similar to the theta rhythm of brain waves related to attention.

Optical illusions reveal regular waves of brain activity enable visual feature integration
High points of brain waves and correct answers occur at similar times. Participants’ theta rhythm brain waves were measured by EEG (conceptually illustrated by orange lines) while an image was flashed before their eyes. Volunteers were more likely to correctly report (black lines) what image they had seen if the image was flashed at the same time as a high point of their attention brain wave. Credit: CC-BY-ND Nakayama and Motoyoshi 2019, originally published in The Journal of Neuroscience

"That led us to want to study rhythms of attention and visual perception in more detail," said Motoyoshi.

Binding form and color in conscious perception

In their recent experiments, researchers asked participants to push a button to begin each trial, whenever they felt ready. Then after some unpredictable wait time, two images would quickly flash on a computer screen, one after the other. Participants then reported which images they saw.

"These tests are considered high cognitive load tasks. You have to identify both the —white or black color—and the orientation of the image—left or right diagonal lines," said Ryohei Nakayama, Ph.D., previously a project researcher in Motoyoshi's lab and a currently a researcher at the University of Sydney, Australia.

You can't see what you don't pay attention to

The wait time between the button push and the image flash was between 50 to 800 milliseconds long. In that length of time, theta rhythm brain waves can fluctuate four to seven times.

Example of visual perception test used in the study by University of Tokyo researchers. Volunteers would push a button and then at some unpredictable length of time afterwards (between 50 and 800 milliseconds) two images would briefly flash onscreen. Volunteers then had to report the color (black or white) and orientation (left or right diagonal lines) of the two images they had seen. This video shows a slowed-down version of the test and the almost actual speed of the test. Volunteers performed up to 2,000 individual trials of the test. Credit: CC-BY Images by Ryohei Nakayama

Participants were much more likely to report correctly which images they saw when the images flashed at the same time as a high point of their attention brain waves, as measured by an EEG (electroencephalogram). The opposite was also true: Participants were more likely to give an incorrect answer when the images were flashed at the same time as a low point of their attention brain wave.

Theoretically, an image could become functionally invisible if it always flashed in time with the low points of theta rhythm attention brain waves.

"Under certain conditions, attention is needed to achieve unified perception. That's the classical story," said Motoyoshi.

"But, it is unlikely that one specific brain region is important for attention. Instead, we think the new story is that periodic rhythmic of brain activity are required for both visual perception and ," he continued.

Motoyoshi and Nakayama hope that their ongoing visual research will continue to reveal the complexities of consciousness.

More information: Ryohei Nakayama et al. Attention periodically binds visual features as single events depending on neural oscillations phase-locked to action, The Journal of Neuroscience (2019). DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2494-18.2019

Ryohei Nakayama et al. Discretized Theta-Rhythm Perception Revealed by Moving Stimuli, Scientific Reports (2018). DOI: 10.1038/s41598-018-24131-6

Citation: Optical illusions reveal that regular waves of brain activity enable visual feature integration (2019, May 21) retrieved 28 March 2024 from https://medicalxpress.com/news/2019-05-optical-illusions-reveal-regular-brain.html
This document is subject to copyright. Apart from any fair dealing for the purpose of private study or research, no part may be reproduced without the written permission. The content is provided for information purposes only.

Explore further

Pay attention – how the brain performs a background scan to help focus

81 shares

Feedback to editors